Roku 3 Review, As Promised Early Last Week

Image lifted directly from Roku.com, but it's take an awful lot of chutzpah for them to complain about it.

Image lifted directly from Roku.com, but it’d take an awful lot of arrogance for them to dare complain about it.

 

Both CYC Headquarters (unincorporated Clark County, NV) and CYC Winter Headquarters (Napili-Honokowai, HI) have what’s best styled as an acceptable level of technology. No video game system (thanks, but we’re adults), no 3D TV, no wireless subwoofers (alas, ours are wired.) The general rule is that any technological advancements should ease our lives, not further complicate them. An integrated car stereo that lets you play music and take phone calls at the touch of a button or through voice activation? Sure, if the price is right. An inflatable movie screen? Come on. There’s a reason why The Sharper Image went out of business.

The corollary to the general rule is that good technology should reduce if not eliminate clutter. Many years ago, the fat, disgusting, tobacco-addicted and genitally odoriferous ex-spouse of a CYC principal bought a living room wall’s worth of VHS movies, most of which never even made it out of the shrinkwrap. A decade ago, a musicophile had to lug one’s entire CD collection into the vehicle in order to satisfy eclectic tastes. (There’s nothing more adorable than seeing a modern-day car with the CD sleeve behind the sun visor. The driver can choose among 8 of her favorites!)

Having been availed of Apple TV, Google Chromecast etc., we decided to enter the world of digital video players. We checked out Roku’s latest iteration, the Roku 3; bought one, and have yet to regret our decision.

The device itself is an unassuming flat black cuboid, a Piet Mondrian hockey puck. This particular model, $89 on Amazon, is Roku’s only one designed specifically for HDTVs. Out of the gate we should mention that $89 is the cost in its entirety: there are no monthly fees, usage charges, shakedowns or anything like that. As for the unit itself, among other capabilities it lets your TV access streaming services (Pandora, Hulu, Netflix, and hundreds upon hundreds more.) From our experience, Pandora on an iPhone is swell, Pandora on a laptop can be unwieldy, but Pandora on a living room big screen is fantastic.

The Roku availed us of similar services that we didn’t know existed. M-Go? Sounds reasonable. HBO Go? Sure, why not? Flixster, Pop Flix, ADC, Viewster, Pub-D-Hub, and at this point we think they’re just screwing with us. At least half of these names have got to be dummy services – a Potemkin village of entertainment choices for the modern palate.

And that’s just for viewing. The listening services include dozens more choices that outpace the human ear’s capacity. There are even choices so arcane that just knowing they exist is entertainment enough: like CHN Network, “dedicated to covering news within Canada and the Caribbean community.”

But here’s the trippy part- the Roku feature that we use most often isn’t even listed among its major selling points. Roku can play your music collection and/or your photo collection directly from your iPhone (or Android phone, presumably.) The “and/or” designation is critical, too. You can play your music through your TV’s speakers while the screen features a slideshow (or still shot) of your choice. Or you can just play the photos. Or you can just play the music. (If you do, the visual accompaniment is the artist/album/song information and associated artwork, bouncing around in screensaver fashion.)

Better still, Roku makes Google Play and iPhone apps that turn your phone into a remote. From personal experience, it works perfectly. Even better than that, this does not render superfluous the remote that comes with the Roku box itself. That remote has a headphone jack. Which means that if one person wants to watch or listen to something grating and cacophonous, with a pair of headphones the remainder of the household can be spared.

Built-in wireless. Ethernet if that’s your thing. Motion control for games, and something about Angry Birds (sorry, we started wandering off when talk turned to video games.) The Roku 3 includes a microSD card slot and a USB port, too. That’s in addition to the HDMI cable port you need to get the thing working. Once you’re up and running you can search for movies, TV shows etc. via Roku’s comprehensive on-screen menu. You will never again go back to the cumbersome method of selecting various services on your computer and looking at them individually.

The only downside is that Roku can’t play DRM-protected content from, say, the iTunes store. And if you heeded our advice and bought music from a Russian knock-off digital media store that offers a wider selection than iTunes does and sells everything at 90% off Apple’s prices, that’d be a non-issue.

When we think of how as recently as 2008 we hired a crooked audio/video installer to spend weeks wiring our home with superfluous services such as multiple iPhone ports throughout the house (we needed only one, and now that Apple made the iPhone 1/3″ higher and incorporated the 8-pin Lightning dock connector, even that one port is useless), it makes us want to cry. Being able to play music from an iPhone mounted on the wall used to be the height of luxury. Now it’s outmoded, superseded by our newfound ability to play music, movies, and probably even porn directly through our TV. Thank you, Jesus, and thank you, Roku.

This was not a compensated post. The folks at Roku didn’t give us so much as a single shiny farthing. But this is a personal finance site, and we figured it was worth your while to know that you can buy what once would have been hundreds if not thousands of dollars worth of entertainment features and capabilities for a mere $89 (slightly more if ordered directly from the company.)

That $75,000 Thing Is A Crock Of Garbage

"You can take the rest of these back. I only want the one."

“You can take the rest of these back. I only want the one.”

This is what happens when you cede your thinking to journalists and let them write your headlines for you. Misinterpretation abounds and becomes calcified as fact.

4 years ago, a famous study appeared that was said to conclude that the optimal salary for human happiness was $75,000. No word on what the optimal effective tax rate, optimal local cost of living or optimal amount of expenses were, but you can’t expect the media to understand concepts that have multiple layers. By the way, we could have attached any of a million other links, but simpletons still regard Time magazine as some sort of authority for some reason. Actually, what the hell: here are some more, running the political continuum all the way from the Wall Street Journal to the New York Times. It should go without saying that every unimaginative personal finance blogger wrote her own evaluation of the study, too.

Happiness tracks salary up to $75,000, then it either levels off or declines. If you’re not skeptical when you read something like that, ask yourself why you aren’t. There isn’t an employee anywhere on Earth making >$75,000 who has demanded a pay cut to the so-called optimal level. From real-world observation, we know plenty of people making much more than $75,000 who seem to be having the time of their lives. So wherefore the study?

One of the 2 principals behind the study is Daniel Kahneman, the 2002 Nobel economics laureate and all-around smart guy. His 2011 book Thinking Fast And Slow is illustrative nonfiction, lay cognitive psychology for people who have graduated past Malcolm Gladwell and have a hankering for something more rigorous. We bought the book recently, knowing of Kahneman but unaware that he was half the brains behind the $75,000 study. Reading the book, we realized that everyone completely missed the point. 

Kahneman did reference $75,000/year as being a maximum of sorts, and makes repeated use of his own idiomatic term “experienced well-being” in conjunction with the $75,000 figure, but he didn’t say that $75,000/year was the ideal salary for human happiness or make anything close to such a wild and counterintuitive proclamation. What he said takes more words to explain than there are in a typical headline.

In any group of people, there will be some with feelings of despair. Unsurprisingly, the poorer the group is, the greater the proportion of people in it who have said feelings. This isn’t exactly news. Maybe 19% of people in a high income bracket and 38% of people in a low income bracket feel these negative thoughts, according to Kahneman’s research.

Now add a mildly deleterious event, like a headache. That’s enough to turn even more people despondent, and it does. The headache will increase the proportion of rich people who feel depressed, and it will do the same for the poor people. But it’ll do the latter to a greater extent. Compound their situation with a headache, and an additional 19% of rich people will be sad and/or worried. Add that same headache to the subset of poor people, and an additional 32% of them will acknowledge feelings of depression. (All numbers taken from Thinking Fast and Slow.) A minus, even one that a Tylenol or two will cure within hours, slams the poor worse than it does the rich. Again, these discoveries don’t seem all that revelatory, at least to us.

Now, imagine a positive event of similar absolute value. A hole-in-one, your kid coming home with a report card full of straight A’s, a stranger paying you a compliment, whatever. That ought to improve your mood no matter how much money you make. Among poor people, the percentage of them whom such an event will put in a bouncy mood will increase. Among rich people, most of whom are happier than the poor folk to begin with, the numbers will also increase when the positive event happens. But of course, this is bounded. If 99% of people who make $1 million a year are generally ecstatic to begin with, all the strokes of good fortune in the world aren’t going to increase the ranks of such people by more than a percentage point.

What Kahneman said was that the biggest increase in well-being happens around $75,000 a year. Mathematically speaking, that’s where dy/dx, the 1st derivative of the curve, is its highest. If you’re an adult making $20,000/year, chances are relatively strong (relative to people who make higher incomes) that your outlook on life is going to be pretty bleak. A random positive event will increase the percentage of people at that income level who feel happy and optimistic, but not by that much – only a few percentage points. Conversely, at the upper end of income, the random positive event will indeed turn some frowns upside-down – but at that level, the frowns are rare to begin with. $75,000 marks the point at which a positive occurrence makes the largest difference, moves the most people from morose or lukewarm into bags of joy. As Kahneman says,

Being poor makes one miserable, and…being rich may enhance one’s life satisfaction, but does not (on average) improve experienced well-being.

Which requires an explanation of the difference between “life satisfaction” and “experienced well-being.” He defines the latter as your day-to-day or minute-to-minute answer to “How are you feeling?” The day that the guards bring the monthly allocation of bean paste to the prisoners at Pukchang prison camp is great, relative to the 30 that surround it. “Life satisfaction” is something a little more broad: “the satisfaction of the remembering self.” And that, rich people have an abundance of.

So in short, Kahneman said that if you were to quantify one’s reaction to a good thing happening, the important variables are 1) the event itself and b) the person’s starting point. And a modest starting point will mean a greater increase in “experienced well-being.” Think about it: who gets a bigger thrill from hitting a home run – the 20-year-old just called up from Toledo and making his major league debut, or Albert Pujols, who’s already done this thing 492 times? That doesn’t mean that the guy fighting to stay on a big-league roster is somehow in a better position than a 3-time MVP and future Hall of Famer.

To the lay person’s ears, distinguishing between experienced well-being and life satisfaction might seem like nothing more than coming with unnecessary alternate phrasings for “happiness”. But there’s a difference. One is marginal, the other cumulative. Cumulative is better. It’s “he who dies with the most toys” who wins, not “he who has the greatest temporary increase in happiness with the addition of a particular toy.”  Debunking these stupid myths is fun.